ZME Science
No Result
View All Result
ZME Science
No Result
View All Result
ZME Science

Home → Features → Health → Mind and Brain

Do we think in words or pictures?

It's a mix of both and much more.

Joshika KomarlabyJoshika Komarla
February 23, 2024 - Updated on February 24, 2024
in Mind and Brain
A A
Edited and reviewed by Tibi Puiu
Share on FacebookShare on TwitterSubmit to Reddit
Man looking at white board
Not all thought is the same. Image credits: Christina Morillo.

Imagine an elephant. What happens inside your mind? As you processed the previous sentence, you might have visualized the animal, or you may have heard your inner voice utter the word. Some of you may have even experienced a sudden emotional response—happiness, sadness, or perhaps even fear—arising from the mental imagery of this admittedly large herbivore.

Thought is tricky. It is, by and large, a universal experience, yet at the same time, innately subjective. No two people think identically. However, there are a few general cognitive patterns common to us all. For instance, most people fall under two major modes of thought: we either engage in verbal thinking, where our inner dialogue takes center stage, and/or visual thinking, where mental images play a key role in our cognition.

The Battle of the Brain: verbal versus visual

It’s an age-old question: do we think in pictures or words? For the longest time, human cognition has primarily been conceived in two modes of thought: verbal and visual. Think about that ever-chatty inner voice — the one that never seems to take a break from narrating your life. Chances are, if you’ve got a persistent verbal commentator in your head, you’re leaning toward being a verbal thinker.

What does verbal thinking feel like?

Internal voices are sort of like personal guides for verbal thinkers. In an article on LiveScience, Dr. Hélène Lœvenbruck, a neurolinguistics researcher, explains this internal dialogue.

“By inner monologue, we mean that we can have private speech that’s addressed to ourselves and that is carried out without any articulation or sound,” opined Dr. Lœvenbruck.

People experience this monologue in various ways. Some hear thoughts in their voices, while others tune in to an impersonal, all-knowing narrator. And then, some play host to more than one voice. Imagine this scenario: an individual whose inner voices take the form of an elderly Italian couple. This intriguing case was highlighted in a Guardian article and sheds light on the diverse nature of verbal thought processes.

“I have no idea where this has come from. It’s probably offensive to Italians,” explained the person in question.

RelatedPosts

This is what ocean acidification is doing to creatures in the Arctic
“Clean coal is a lie,” coal CEO admits
How to milk a potato? Start-up grows dairy protein inside potatoes
France’s ban on unhealthily skinny models just came into force

“They passionately argue either side,” she says. “It’s really useful because I let them do the work, so I don’t get stressed out by it.”

Such eccentricities highlight the many dimensions of verbal thinking. Dr. Lœvenbruck there are three main dimensions of verbal thought: dialogality, condensation, and intentionality.

The frontal and temporal regions of the brain are primarily involved in verbal thinking.
The frontal and temporal regions of the brain are primarily involved in verbal thinking. Image via Wiki Commons.

Dialogality

Let’s rewind to the example of the person whose inner voice — or rather, voices — is an old Italian couple. This person reported that the voices often chatted and even bickered with one another. This quality of multiple voices conversing with one another refers to the dialogality of our inner voices. Essentially, our inner voice isn’t just the first 10 minutes of a late-night show. We’re capable of having full-fledged conversations in our brains.

Condensation

Inner voices aren’t Shakespearean. They aren’t particularly verbose or composed either. A lot of the time, inner voices are incoherent and messy. We switch between fleeting, scattered thoughts to fully developed essays. This is the inner voice’s condensation in action.

Intentionality

Verbal thinking isn’t always an entirely active process. Sometimes, thoughts just pop up. Out of the blue, your inner narrator can start up a conversation with itself. This quality, involving the control of our thoughts, is known as intentionality.

How common is the “inner voice”?

Traditionally, verbal thought has long been billed as the ‘primary’ mode of thought but it’s essential to recognize that not everyone thinks this way. Studies suggest that only approximately 30 to 50 percent of individuals perceive their thoughts through inner speech. Even among those who do, they don’t exclusively perceive their thoughts verbally. Verbal thinking doesn’t exist in isolation. Visualization often accompanies inner speech. For instance, try writing something down. While you can certainly hear yourself narrate the words, some can even visualize the sentences and the individual words coming together in the mind’s eye.

When do we use visual thinking?

Consider:

  • Your loyal canine companion
  • Your siblings or parents
  • Your latest beach excursion
  • Messi’s iconic World Cup victory in Qatar
  • Your go-to comfort food

Images of our drooly best friends, cherished memories with the family, Messi’s unadulterated joy at finally lifting the Cup, and the comforting allure of a favorite dish, like a Sunday roast, flood your brain. Thoughts like these, come to life in our brain. We can see them in the eye of our mind.

Scenery of a beach
Attributes of episodic memory, like memories and places, are best accessed through visual thinking. Image credits: Asad Photo Maldives.

Long-term memory

Visual thinking is an ancient mode of cognition. It predates verbal thinking and is generally the go-to process for the brain when it comes to processes like episodic memory — a type of long-term memory. Autobiographical thoughts — things like personal experiences, specific life events, places, and objects — are largely perceived as pictures or images, as opposed to inner speech. When people reminisce about stuff, they often “see” the scenes and details in their mind’s eye. This visual aspect of episodic memory highlights the role of visual thinking in capturing and reliving personal experiences. Similarly, visual thinking plays a massive role in other mental processes like object manipulation and self-projection.

Object manipulation

Object manipulation refers to the mental process of interacting with physical objects. It’s how we catch a ball in mid-flight or shape dough. This process serves as the bridge between our intent to act and the dexterous control over our hands (or other body parts) to grasp, move, shape, or otherwise influence the position or state of an object.

Someone playing basketball near a hoop
Instances involving hand-eye coordination, like sports, are good examples of object manipulation. Image credits: Pixabay.

Object manipulation can range from basic activities of daily living — like picking up our toothbrush, to more intricate tasks, such as typing out words using a keyboard — that require a high level of manual dexterity and coordination. The brain’s ability to coordinate visual, tactile, and motor information is crucial for successful object manipulation.

Self-projection

When thinking about the future, do we picture or hear future us? This cognitive mechanism we use to explore and plan for future possibilities, foster behaviors, and set goals for ourselves is known as self-projection. Similar to the intentionality dimension of verbal thinking, self-projection is precisely what it sounds like — an endeavor to mentally project ourselves into situations; to perceive ourselves in our minds.

It isn’t inherently limited to any one mode of thinking.

For instance, while some aspects of projection involve a verbal component, such as verbal affirmations, a significant portion of self-projection is visual. Mentally simulating future scenarios, imagining oneself in different situations, and picturing the outcomes of one’s actions are all robust examples of utilizing visual thinking to facilitate self-projection.

The interplay of verbal and visual thinking

People aren’t as one-dimensional as they might believe. Even those who strongly identify as verbal thinkers may, at times, inadvertently engage in visual thinking. Processes like self-projection provide windows into the inner workings of the human mind. No single brain is inherently either verbal or visual. Instead, there exists an interplay between the two modes to coherently process all human thought.

Interesting questions raised by science

In a bid to investigate this interplay, researchers from Brown University, Harvard Medical School, MIT, and the Massachusetts General Hospital set up a study to explore the extent of overlap between verbal and visual thinking. In an article with the Harvard Gazette, lead researcher Elinor Amit elaborated on their line of questioning.

“The question we wanted to answer was: Can you engage in one without the other modality popping up?” Amit said. “Can you use one without invoking the other unintentionally?” explained Amit.

Amit and her team conducted two experiments to delve into the dynamics of verbal and visual thinking. The first, a behavioral experiment conducted in lab settings, involved tasks where participants engaged in verbal and visual thinking based on prompts and were asked to silently imagine a sentence or mental picture. The second experiment was a bit more technical and used functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) machines to scrutinize neural activity in the brain generated as a result of given tasks.

The occipital lobe is primarily involved with visual thinking
The occipital lobe is primarily involved with visual thinking. Image via Wiki Commons.

After completing the tasks, all participants were asked four questions:

  • How clear were the images they created?
  • How clear were the sentences they created?
  • How clear were any images they unintentionally created?
  • How clear were any sentences they unintentionally created?

Amit explained the type of tasks and the subsequent line of questioning posed to the participants.

“So in one trial you might be asked to create an image, and we would ask you how clear that image was. In the next trial, we might ask you to create an image again, but then ask you how clear was the sentence you unintentionally created,” clarified Amit.

What did they discover? Let’s find out.

Insights from research

Contrary to very popular belief, verbal and visual thinking are not independent processes. The study unveiled an asymmetrical relationship between the two, highlighting that even when intentionally engaging in verbal thought processes, visual thinking almost always emerges as a by-product.

Imagine needing to stock up on essentials and creating a grocery list. As you mentally list the items (verbal thinking), your mind simultaneously conjures images of the products on the shelves (visual thinking). For instance, if you jot down ‘orange juice,’ not only do you hear yourself say the words, but a visual of a glass or gallon of OJ also pops up in your mind.

This wasn’t all the study came up with. Not only are the processes intertwined, but they also operate at varying levels. Generally, verbal processes are more pronounced and verbose—especially when intentional. Visual thinking, on the other hand, operates at the same level of intensity regardless of intent.

While verbal thinking can lead to more pronounced, intricate, and elaborate thoughts based on our effort, visual thinking presents a different dynamic. Contrary to prior belief, the images in our heads aren’t produced in high definition. It isn’t a continuous movie but rather a series of hazy stills.

“We found that people generated more robust verbal representations during deliberate inner speech … but they generated visual images regardless of whether their intent was to visualize something or to think verbally,” explained Amit.

The study not only clarified the overlap between the two processes but also assigned evolutionary roots to them. The prevalence of visual thinking in our minds has a purpose – language evolved long after our capacity to process stimuli. That is, to think!

Conclusion

In the realm of cognition, phenomenology, and psychology, questions as seemingly straightforward as ‘Do we think in pictures or words?’ defy easy answers. Contrary to popular belief, even the most eloquent thinkers aren’t exclusively verbal, and the same goes for visual thinkers. Perhaps a more insightful inquiry is, ‘Which situations or processes prompt extensive verbal or visual thinking?’ However, even this line of questioning seems simplistic, considering that visual thinking, albeit low clarity, consistently lurks in the recesses of our minds.

GPS navigation in a car
For tasks like navigation, we use spatial thinking. Image via Wiki Commons.

The human mind is a vast expanse that cannot operate within the confines of just two modalities of thought. For instance, let’s consider the example of spatial thinking — one of the many thought processes of the human brain. With spatial thinking, we’re talking about comprehending space. This facilitates an understanding of spatial configurations, such as distances, angles, and orientations. Such thought processes become vital when we have to navigate our surroundings. Imagine, sitting shotgun and navigating. While you may think you’re engaging your visual faculties — after all; you are using your eyes and picturing the route to your destination — you aren’t just thinking visually. Mostly, you’re thinking spatially.

Research from 2008 identified a link between verbal thinking and spatial thinking, especially in certain situations, such as when solving problems related to geometry. In problem-solving scenarios such as these, despite the obvious visual elements (geometrical shapes), it isn’t just enough to picture them. Deliberate and systematic thought processes are required. In such contexts, it becomes increasingly apparent that although visual and verbal thinking are predominant patterns, they don’t represent the full scope of human thought.

ShareTweetShare
Joshika Komarla

Joshika Komarla

Joshika Komarla is a wildlife conservationist and science writer based in Bangalore, India. Armed with a triple major in zoology, botany, and chemistry, she pursued scientific communication to fulfil her lifelong fascination with all questions silly and obscure. Her downtime (read: all her time) is spent protecting innocent cats and birds from her pup, Nala.

Related Posts

Health

Muscle bros love their cold plunges. Science says they don’t really work (for gains)

byMihai Andrei
2 hours ago
Archaeology

Scientists Reconstruct The Face of a 400-year-old Polish ‘Vampire’

byTibi Puiu
3 hours ago
Genetics

Revolutionary single-dose cholesterol treatment could reduce levels by up to 69%

byMihai Andrei
4 hours ago
Mind & Brain

Your Brain Uses Only 5% More Energy Whether You’re Actively Thinking or Not. So, What Causes Mental Fatigue?

byTibi Puiu
4 hours ago

Recent news

Muscle bros love their cold plunges. Science says they don’t really work (for gains)

June 9, 2025

Scientists Reconstruct The Face of a 400-year-old Polish ‘Vampire’

June 9, 2025

Revolutionary single-dose cholesterol treatment could reduce levels by up to 69%

June 9, 2025
  • About
  • Advertise
  • Editorial Policy
  • Privacy Policy and Terms of Use
  • How we review products
  • Contact

© 2007-2025 ZME Science - Not exactly rocket science. All Rights Reserved.

No Result
View All Result
  • Science News
  • Environment
  • Health
  • Space
  • Future
  • Features
    • Natural Sciences
    • Physics
      • Matter and Energy
      • Quantum Mechanics
      • Thermodynamics
    • Chemistry
      • Periodic Table
      • Applied Chemistry
      • Materials
      • Physical Chemistry
    • Biology
      • Anatomy
      • Biochemistry
      • Ecology
      • Genetics
      • Microbiology
      • Plants and Fungi
    • Geology and Paleontology
      • Planet Earth
      • Earth Dynamics
      • Rocks and Minerals
      • Volcanoes
      • Dinosaurs
      • Fossils
    • Animals
      • Mammals
      • Birds
      • Fish
      • Amphibians
      • Reptiles
      • Invertebrates
      • Pets
      • Conservation
      • Animal facts
    • Climate and Weather
      • Climate change
      • Weather and atmosphere
    • Health
      • Drugs
      • Diseases and Conditions
      • Human Body
      • Mind and Brain
      • Food and Nutrition
      • Wellness
    • History and Humanities
      • Anthropology
      • Archaeology
      • History
      • Economics
      • People
      • Sociology
    • Space & Astronomy
      • The Solar System
      • Sun
      • The Moon
      • Planets
      • Asteroids, meteors & comets
      • Astronomy
      • Astrophysics
      • Cosmology
      • Exoplanets & Alien Life
      • Spaceflight and Exploration
    • Technology
      • Computer Science & IT
      • Engineering
      • Inventions
      • Sustainability
      • Renewable Energy
      • Green Living
    • Culture
    • Resources
  • Videos
  • Reviews
  • About Us
    • About
    • The Team
    • Advertise
    • Contribute
    • Editorial policy
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact

© 2007-2025 ZME Science - Not exactly rocket science. All Rights Reserved.

OSZAR »